Calder, Andrew J.Jenkins, RobCassel, AnneliClifford, Colin W. G. (n.d.). Visual representation of eye gaze is coded by a nonopponent multichannel system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(7), 244–261. https://search.proquest.com/docview/614481757?accountid=14782
Carmel, D., Arcaro, M., Kastner, S., & Hasson, U. (2010). How to Create and Use Binocular Rivalry. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 45. https://doi.org/10.3791/2030
David A. Leopold ; Alice J. O’Toole ; Thomas Vetter ; Volker Blanz. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/82947
Deheane, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le Bihan, D., Mangin, J.-F., Poline, J.-B., & Riviere, D. (n.d.). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. https://www.nature.com/articles/nn0701_752.pdf
Fang, F., & He, S. (2005). Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways. Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 1380–1385. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1537
Goldstein, E. B. (2014). Sensation and perception (Ninth edition). Wadsworth.
Hakwan C. Lau and Richard E. Passingham. (2006). Relative Blindsight in Normal Observers and the Neural Correlate of Visual Consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(49). https://www.jstor.org/stable/30051187?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents
Haynes, J.-D., & Rees, G. (2005). Predicting the orientation of invisible stimuli from activity in human primary visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 8(5), 686–691. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1445
Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 197–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9
Mark A. Williams. (2004). Amygdala Responses to Fearful and Happy Facial Expressions under Conditions of Binocular Suppression. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(12), 2898–2904. http://www.jneurosci.org/content/24/12/2898
Newsome, W. T., Britten, K. H., & Movshon, J. A. (1989). Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature, 341(6237), 52–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/341052a0
R. W. Kentridge, C. A. Heywood and L. Weiskrantz. (1999). Attention without Awareness in Blindsight. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 266(1430). https://www.jstor.org/stable/51579?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents
Robert J. Snowden. (2012). Basic vision. Oxford University Press. https://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/vuw/detail.action?docID=1591383
Sejnowski, Terrence J., E., David. M. (5460). Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science., 287(5460), 2036–2038. http://tewaharoa.victoria.ac.nz/primo_library/libweb/action/openurl?aulast=Eagleman&isSerivcesPage=true&dscnt=2&auinit=DM&atitle=Motion+integration+and+postdiction+in+visual+awareness&url_ctx_fmt=null&sid=google&vid=VUW_SERVICES_PAGE&institution=64VUW&id=pmid%3A10720334&dstmp=1469479506527&fromLogin=true
Stephen M. Fleming, Rimona S. Weil, Zoltan Nagy, Raymond J. Dolan and Geraint Rees. (2010). Relating Introspective Accuracy to Individual Differences in Brain Structure. Science, 329(5998). https://www.jstor.org/stable/40803109?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents
Sterzer, P., Haynes, J. D., & Rees, G. (2008). Fine-scale activity patterns in high-level visual areas encode the category of invisible objects. Journal of Vision, 8(15), 10–10. https://doi.org/10.1167/8.15.10
Susilo, Tirta ; Mckone, Elinor ; Edwards, Mark. (2010). Solving the upside-down puzzle: Why do upright and inverted face aftereffects look alike? Journal Of Vision, 10(13). https://doi.org/10.1167/10.13.1
Whitney, D., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: a fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 160–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005
Wolfe, J. M. (n.d.). Sensation & perception (3rd ed). Sinauer Associates.